Indan Journal of Medical Research Indan Journal of Medical Research Indan Journal of Medical Research Indan Journal of Medical Research
  Home About us Editorial board Search Ahead of print Current issue Archives Submit article Instructions Subscribe Contacts Login  
  Home Print this page Email this page Small font sizeDefault font sizeIncrease font size Users Online: 131       

   Table of Contents      
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Year : 2012  |  Volume : 136  |  Issue : 6  |  Page : 1025-1030

Inhibition of self-grooming induced by sleep restriction in dam rats


1 Department of Psychobiology-Universidede Federal São Paulo-São, Paulo, Brazil
2 Department of Physiological Sciences- Universidade Federal de Goiás - Goiânia, Brazil

Date of Submission03-Jan-2011
Date of Web Publication4-Feb-2013

Correspondence Address:
Monica Levy Andersen
Department of Psychobiology, Universidade Federal de São Paulo, Rua Napoleão de Barros, 925, Vila Clementino - SP 04021-002, São Paulo
Brazil
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


PMID: 23391800

Rights and PermissionsRights and Permissions
   Abstract 

Background & objectives : Sleep restriction is a common feature of modern lifestyle and its effects can be extended to pregnancy. Several neurobehavioural consequences of sleep restriction during pregnancy have been reported, among which stand out perinatal depression and maternal fatigue, however, its effects over mother-infant relationship warrant further investigation. Thus, this study was aimed to evaluate the effects of sleep restriction during pregnancy over maternal behaviour and maternal aggression through animal models.
Methods: Eighteen 90-day-old female Wistar rats were distributed in two groups: (i) Control - not submitted to any manipulation during pregnancy, and (ii) Sleep restriction - submitted to sleep restriction during the entire pregnancy (21 days) through the multiple platforms technique. In the postpartum day 5, resident-intruder paradigm and the latencies test were performed to assess both maternal behaviour and maternal aggression.
Results: The sleep-restricted females displayed grooming in less frequency and duration, and with higher latency when compared to normal animals, while maternal aggression and maternal behaviour parameters remained equivalent between groups.
Interpretation & conclusions : Considering the maintenance of maternal behavioural parameters, the inhibition of grooming seems to exert an adaptive mechanism, enabling sleep-restricted rats to display maternal behaviour properly.

Keywords: Grooming - maternal behavior - pregnancy - sleep restriction


How to cite this article:
Pires GN, Alvarenga TA, Maia LO, Mazaro-Costa R, Tufik S, Andersen ML. Inhibition of self-grooming induced by sleep restriction in dam rats. Indian J Med Res 2012;136:1025-30

How to cite this URL:
Pires GN, Alvarenga TA, Maia LO, Mazaro-Costa R, Tufik S, Andersen ML. Inhibition of self-grooming induced by sleep restriction in dam rats. Indian J Med Res [serial online] 2012 [cited 2019 Sep 16];136:1025-30. Available from: http://www.ijmr.org.in/text.asp?2012/136/6/1025/106866

Pregnancy is a physiological process and numerous physiological and anatomical changes take place in the body to sustain it. Even in normal conditions, this state predisposes women to lack of sleep, among other consequences [1],[2],[3] . During gestation, several sleep complaints and polysomnographic findings become common, ranging from hypersomnia and decreased slow-wave sleep (first trimester) to insomnia, non-restful sleep and diminished rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (third trimester) [2] . These complaints are mainly due to the marked anatomo-physiological changes inherent to this period in addition to alterations of hormone secretion, greater urinary flow and frequency, difficulty in assuming non habitual sleep positions, among other [1] .

The poor sleep quality related to pregnant women becomes more of a cause of concern when contextualized to modern living standards [3],[4] . The lifestyle has changed affecting women more severely than man due to added social and professional demands [5] . This situation is aggravated during pregnancy, as pregnant women are subjected to insufficient sleep, promoted by intrinsic gestation circumstances and to impositions of modern lifestyle [3],[6] .

Sleep curtailment in pregnant women has been associated with long-term impairment and negative outcomes after the parturition [4],[7] . Sleep loss during pregnancy leads to neurobehavioural and psychiatric co-morbidities, including perinatal depression [7],[8] and maternal fatigue [9] . However, little has been documented about the postpartum consequences induced by sleep deprivation over the offspring as well as over parental behaviour. Recently, Pires et al[4] have hypothesized that sleep impairment during pregnancy could lead to deficient mother-infant relationship. However, this concept warrants further investigation as it was theoretically discussed, but not experimentally assayed.

Animal models of maternal behaviour are useful experimental tools to understand the mechanisms underlying mother-infant relationship. However, the studies regarding the interface between maternal behaviour and sleep are incipient [10] . Thus, this study was aimed to examine the effects of sleep restriction during pregnancy over two important issues related to parental behaviour - maternal behaviour per se and maternal aggression in rats.


   Material & Methods Top


This study was conducted in the Department of Psychobiology, University of Sγo Paulo, Sγo Paulo, Brazil. For this experiment 18, 90-day old female Wistar rats were obtained from the Center for Development of Experimental Models for Medicine and Biology (CEDEME), Sao Paulo, Brazil. These subjects were kept in monitored rooms with controlled temperature (22±1°C) and a 12 h light-dark cycle. The experimental protocol was approved by the institutional research ethics committee.

The female rats were mated, and pregnancy was confirmed by the presence of spermatozoa in the vaginal smear associated with compatible estrous cycle condition and, when possible, by the visualization of a vaginal plug. In the first day of pregnancy, the female rats were distributed in two groups: (i) Control - not submitted to any manipulation during pregnancy, and (ii) Sleep restriction - submitted to sleep restriction during the entire pregnancy (21 days). The control group for this experiment was in accordance to previous studies on maternal sleep deprivation through the platform method. Briefly, this protocol consisted of subjecting the animals to prolonged wakefulness for 18 h per day (from 1600 - 1000h) by the multiple platforms technique, for 21 days. During the remaining 6 h, the animals were returned to their home-cages and allowed to sleep. Sleep restriction was conducted in a tiled water tank (110x41x30cm), which contained 14 platforms, 6.5 cm in diameter and rising 1 cm above water surface [11] . The day of the partum was defined as postpartum day 0 (PPD0) and in the PPD1 the litters were standardized in 8 pups. The dams were subjected to behavioural tests to assess maternal behaviour and maternal aggressive behaviour at PPD3 and PPD5, which corresponds to the ascending phase of these behaviours [12],[13] . The protocol was adapted from earlier studies [14],[15] and consisted of two different tests, as described below:

Resident-intruder paradigm: This test was performed to assess both aggressive maternal behaviour and maternal behaviour in the presence of an intruder rat. Thus, at the PPD3 and PPD5, a naive 60-day old male rat was introduced in the home-cage with the female rat and its respective litter. This test was able to quantify latency, frequency and duration of behavioral patterns related to maternal behaviour, maternal aggressive behaviour and non-maternal behaviours [Table 1].
Table 1. Results of comparison between control and sleep deprived females


Click here to view


Latencies test: This test was specific to maternal behaviour. Ten minutes after the end of the resident-intruder paradigm, the female rats were separated from their litter for 5 min. During this time, the litter was sent to the home-cage. When the female rat returned to the cage, the latency to retrieve the first and the last pup was measured. The time between the latencies was calculated.

Both behavioural tests had a cut-off of 10 min and were conducted in the first third of the dark phase. The tests performed at PPD3 were considered as habituation and were not quantified for analysis. The dams were weighed before the parturition and in the PPD5. The results of the tests at PPD5 as well as the dams' weight were compared through the Mann-Whitney test.


   Results Top


The most affected behaviour by sleep restriction during pregnancy was self-grooming, which was inhibited. Grooming was significantly reduced in the sleep restriction group regarding both frequency (4.7 ± 3.5 vs 1.6 ± 1.7; P=0.02) and duration (31.4 ± 24.7s vs 4.4 ± 5.3 sec; P<0.01) in relation to normal sleep animals. In addition, mean latency of self-grooming was significantly higher in sleep-restricted females (83.7 ± 66.4 sec vs 366.2 ± 225.9 sec; P<0.01), indicating that these animals delayed the drive to self-groom in the presence of a stressful stimulus (intruder male). Statistically significant results were also obtained in latency for lateral attack (168.7 ± 182.0 sec vs 71.33 ± 109.8 sec; P=0.04) and frequency for nest building (6.0 ± 6.3 vs 5.0 ± 3.7; P=0.03). There were no significant differences in dam's weight neither before the parturition (336 ± 21.2 vs 322.4±22.8; P=0.45) nor in the PPD5 (276.8 ± 28.1 vs 250.3 ± 15.4 g; P=0.07).


   Discussion Top


The significant results in this study were found in the self-grooming. A decrease was found in lateral attack latency and in nest building frequency in sleep-restricted females, pointing to an increase in the defensive component of maternal aggression and decrease in a maternal care-related behaviour, respectively. The data regarding the lateral attack latency partially corroborated the results of Sandrin and Hoshino [16] , who have described an increase of defensive aggression in sleep-deprived rats. However, the findings related to nest building and lateral attack were isolated and not accompanied by complementary behavioural parameters (latency and duration), in contrast to the results for grooming. The hypothesis that the findings related to lateral attack and nest building were occasional and had no practical relevance can be raised, despite the statistical significance. Thus, both maternal aggression and maternal behaviour in sleep-restricted females were equivalent to the control group, as the majority of parameters related to these behaviours were not significantly different.

The sleep pattern in pregnant rats is similar to what is seen in women, showing dynamic oscillations in total sleep time and in other features [17] . Moreover, the sleep restriction methodology through the platform method is specific to REM sleep [18] , promoting sleep fragmentation rather than total sleep deprivation [11] . Accordingly, REM sleep deprivation provides a better framework to mimic disrupted sleep patterns during pregnancy than does total sleep deprivation because pregnant women show a marked REM sleep curtailment, mostly in the third trimester of gestation [2] . Hence, sleep restriction by the platform method in animals can be taken, with caution, as analogue to the sleep deficit scenario experienced by pregnant women. These results corroborate, in some degree, the hypothesis proposed by Pires et al[4] , since behavioral alterations related to mother-infant relationship were observed as consequence to sleep restriction. However, the behavioral alterations did not occur in maternal behavior per se, but in behavioral adaptive mechanisms, thus enabling effective maternal care.

The data were also analyzed in the perspective of the approach-avoidance model of maternal behavior [19],[20] . This model proposes that the maternal behaviour occurs when the tendency of approach and interaction with the litter is greater than the tendency to avoid pup stimuli. According to this model, maternal behaviour is a result of both maternal motivational and non-specific systems, the interaction between these systems, and the influence of external stimuli over them.

Self-grooming is a highly complex behaviour [21] with a well known association with high stress situations [22] . In rodents, the grooming analysis is an indirect approach to infer about anxiety [23] . In general, the increase in self-grooming, mainly in frequency, composes the behavioural profile of rodents under an anxiogenic condition [23] . Considering the approach-avoidance model, anxiety-like behaviour and stressful conditions could be considere d as possible factors inducing the avoidance of the litter by the dam, or, more generally, anxiety and stress could compose the antagonistic system, avoiding the maternal behaviour as a whole. Since sleep deprivation and sleep restriction are closely related to stress [24],[25] , a clear deficit in maternal behaviour would be expected under these situations, however, it was not observed, suggesting an adaptive mechanism. The anxiolytic condition observed in the sleep restricted rats, evidenced by the increase in grooming latency and decrease in frequency and duration, could be responsible for allowing these females to display both maternal aggression and maternal behaviour properly.

The relationship between sleep and anxiety has been reported as a feature of adaptive behaviour mechanisms [26] . In the context of maternal coping, a marked stress hyporesponsiveness during lactation is described [27] as well as a dramatic reduction in levels of anxiety-like behaviour [28] . These effects seem to be more prominent in the face of environmental challenges [29] . It might be possible that in an acute environmental challenge (the exposure to a male intruder in the resident-intruder paradigm), the anxiolytic conditions expected for lactating rats are even more prominent in those animals subjected to sleep restriction during pregnancy. Thus, the inhibition of self-grooming due to sleep restriction could be interpreted as one of the several adaptive mechanisms by which maternal care toward the litter is modulated in accordance with environmental demand [30] .

There are some methodological limitations in this study. First, one may inquire whether the results could be due to the stress promoted by sleep loss or to the sleep rebound that occurs after the sleep restriction protocol, rather than to sleep loss per se. However, it should be assumed that both sleep rebound and stress are conditionally associated with sleep deprivation. Even if one considers stress and sleep rebound as the causes of the described data, it is acceptable to state that these results are consequences of the context of sleep deprivation. Second, no sleep recording was performed in this study because the surgery and the recording equipment could represent additional sources of stress for the dams, leading to biased results. Also, the present results are strictly related to pregnancy and puerperium contexts and should not be extended to other conditions.

The present data should not be taken as conclusive findings, but rather as background to further investigation. Several aspects in this behaviour still warrant further studies, addressing in particular the extent to which grooming plays an adaptive role. Moreover, these studies could be even more useful if conducted with animal models that allow translational applicability. It must also be pointed out that the maternal bond and parental behaviour are far more complex in humans than in animals. Parental behaviour in humans is inserted into a social context that is not conceivable in animal experimentation. The adaptive responses observed in animals could give rise to neurobehavioural co-morbidities in humans, such as postpartum depression and maternal fatigue. Thus, the acquired data should be extrapolated to human behaviour with caution. Despite these considerations and of the preliminary nature of these findings, the results are of clinical importance regarding the relationship between disrupted sleep during pregnancy and mother-infant relationship.

 
   References Top

1.Brunner DP, Munch M, Biedermann K, Huch R, Huch A, Borbély AA. Changes in sleep and sleep electroencephalogram during pregnancy. Sleep 1994; 17 : 576-82.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.Parry BL, Martínez LF, Maurer EL, López AM, Sorenson D, Meliska CJ. Sleep, rhythms and women's mood. Part I. Menstrual cycle, pregnancy and postpartum. Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10 :129-44.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.Banks S, Dinges DF. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med 2007; 3 : 519-28.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.Pires GN, Anderson ML, Giovenardi M, Tufik S. Sleep impairment during pregnancy: possible implications on mother-infant relationship. Med Hypotheses. 2010; 75 : 578-82.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.Williams K, Kurina LM. The social structure, stress, and women's health. Clin Obstet Gynecol 2002; 45 : 1099-118.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.Driver HS, Shapiro CM. A longitudinal study of sleep stages in young women during pregnancy and postpartum. Sleep 1992; 15 : 449-53.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.Chang JJ, Pien GW, Duntley SP, Macones GA. Sleep deprivation during pregnancy and maternal and fetal outcomes: is there a relationship? Sleep Med Rev 2010; 14 : 107-14.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.Goyal D, Gay C, Lee K. Fragmented maternal sleep is more strongly correlated with depressive symptoms than infant temperament at three months postpartum. Arch Womens Ment Health 2009; 12 : 229-37.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.Meltzer LJ, Mindell JA. Relationship between child sleep disturbances and maternal sleep, mood, and parenting stress: a pilot study. J Fam Psychol 2007; 21 : 67-73.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.Pires GN, Andersen ML, Tufik S. Sleep and behavioral research: overview on global trends and Brazilian relevance to this field. Sleep Sci (In Press).  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.Machado RB, Suchecki D, Tufik S. Comparison of the sleep pattern throughout a protocol of chronic sleep restriction induced by two methods of paradoxical sleep deprivation. Brain Res Bull 2006; 70 : 213-20.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.Mos J, Olivier B. Quantitative and comparative analyses of pro-aggressive actions of benzodiazepines in maternal aggression of rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1989; 97 : 152-3.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.Giovenardi M, Padoin MJ, Cadore LP, Lucion AB. Hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus, oxytocin, and maternal aggression in rats. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1997; 807 : 606-9.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.Lubin DA, Meter KE, Walker CH, Johns JM. Dose-related effects of chronic gestational cocaine treatment on maternal aggression in rats on postpartum days 2, 3, and 5. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2001; 25 : 1403-20.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.Lucion AB, Almeida RMM. On the dual nature of maternal aggression in rats. Aggres Behav 1996; 22 : 365-73.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.Sandrin MF, Hoshino K. Aggression in sleep deprived rats: ethological characterization of agnonistic confrontations as a defensive behavior. Rev Etologia 1999; 1 : 9-18.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.Kimura M, Zhang SQ, Inoué S. An animal model for pregnancy-associated sleep disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1998; 52 : 209-11.   Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.Mallick BN, Singh A. REM sleep loss increases brain excitability: role of noradrenaline and its mechanism of action. Sleep Med Rev 2011; 15 : 165-78.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.Numan M. Motivational systems and the neural circuitry of maternal behavior in the rat. Dev Psychobiol 2007; 49 : 12-21.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.Rosenblatt JS, Mayer AD. An analysis of approach/withdrawal processes in the initiation of maternal behavior in the laboratory rat. In: Hood KE, Greenberg G, Tobach E, editors. Behavioral development. New York: Garland Press; 1995. p. 177-230.   Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.Kalueff AV, Tuohimaa P. The grooming analysis algorithm discriminates between different levels of anxiety in rats: potential utility for neurobehavioural stress research. J Neurosci Methods 2005; 143 : 169-77.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.Katz RJ, Roth KA. Stress induced grooming in the rat - an endorphin mediated syndrome. Neurosci Lett 1979; 13 : 209-12.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.Kalueff AV, Tuohimaa P. Grooming analysis algorithm for neurobehavioural stress research. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 2004; 13 : 151-8.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.Andersen ML, Bignotto M, Machado RB, Tufik S. Different stress modalities result in distinct steroid hormone responses by male rats. Braz J Med Biol Res 2004; 37 : 791-7.  Back to cited text no. 24
    
25.Galvão Mde O, Sinigaglia-Coimbra R, Kawakami SE, Tufik S, Suchecki D. Paradoxical sleep deprivation activates hypothalamic nuclei that regulate food intake and stress response. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34 : 1176-83.  Back to cited text no. 25
    
26.Suchecki D, Machado RB, Tiba PA. Stress-induced sleep rebound: adaptive behavior and mechanisms. Sleep Sci 2009: 2 : 151-60.  Back to cited text no. 26
    
27.Walker CD, Toufexis DJ, Burlet A. Hypothalamic and limbic expression of CRF and vasopressin during lactation: implications for the control of ACTH secretion and stress hyporesponsiveness. Prog Brain Res 2001; 133 : 99-110.  Back to cited text no. 27
    
28.Neumann ID. Alterations in behavioral and neuroendocrine stress coping strategies in pregnant, parturient and lactating rats. Prog Brain Res 2001; 133 : 143-52.  Back to cited text no. 28
    
29.Carter CS, Altemus M, Chrousos GP. Neuroendocrine and emotional changes in the post-partum period. Prog Brain Res 2001; 133 : 241-9.  Back to cited text no. 29
    
30.Champagne F, Meaney MJ. Like mother, like daughter: evidence for non-genomic transmission of parental behavior and stress responsivity. Prog Brain Res 2001; 133 : 287-302.  Back to cited text no. 30
    



 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1]



 

Top
 
 
  Search
 
    Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
    Access Statistics
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  

 
  In this article
    Abstract
    Material & M...
   Results
   Discussion
    References
    Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed683    
    Printed24    
    Emailed1    
    PDF Downloaded276    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal